Ask musicians
and they will tell you how major chords sound happy and the minor ones sound
sad. Do such differences exist for non-musicians? Do we all perceive music as
happy or sad and if yes then is that choice subjective? These are some of the
questions that would be answered in this post.
Yes, we all
perceive music as being happy or sad and that happens even in the absence of
lyrics. Some of the factors that might be responsible in this affective
perception of music and associate music with emotions include:
1. Culture, language and role of
memory
For most non-musicians the context
to music comes with the lyrics and so partly language does define for them
whether music is happy or sad, more so than the major and minor chords.
Another factor
that causes such an association between emotions and music is memory. So, if
there is a happy memory associated with a music piece then it will make you
feel happy and if there are sad memories associated with a musical piece then
it would evoke sad emotions. Familiarity of a musical piece also trigger
positive emotions. Neuroanatomically, it is the hippocampus in the brain that
helps in this association.
Besides these
subjective differences, music without lyrics also evokes emotions. This points
towards presence of universal features in music that transcends culture,
language and ethnicity.
2. Rhythm, beat and tempo in
context with physiological states of body
The first
musical exposure that humans get is the mother's heartbeat, which is rhythmic
in nature. No wonder then any tempo that is near to 72 bpm would have a calming
effect. This can be extrapolated thus to different physiological conditions
corresponding to heart beats. A higher tempo music would sound happy and a
lower tempo would sound sad. But there are studies that suggest otherwise.
3. Consonance and Dissonance,
physical characteristics of sound
The physical
nature of sound itself makes a combination of some tones pleasing to us, while
others less pleasing. Each tone generates multiple other overtones in our ears
due to resonance. Tones that generate overlapping overtones sound pleasant,
called as consonance, as if all the sound waves agree with each other. This is
exactly what major chord is made up of, tones that generate overlapping
overtones. Consonance of music is a universal feature seen as pleasant by not
just human adults, but also by babies and by other species like birds, when
assessed by their preference for consonant music rather than dissonant music.
But sad music is pleasurable too, thus, consonance alone cannot distinguish
these different emotions.
Music evoked movement provides
the final answer. We all can relate to tapping fingers, feet and banging
heads on a beat. This movement which itself gets evoked on listening to music
is another universal feature, as music also activates the motor region of the
brain that is cerebellum.
When a
comparison between movement (by showing a bouncing ball) and music was done
varying different features like beat and consonance, it was found that a different
combination of these factors could define different emotions.
It appears that
a combination of tempo and consonance can define universal emotional states by
activating specific networks in brain consisting of auditory regions,
hippocampus, limbic region and cerebellum. Thus, there is a different formula
for happy and sad music, as each can literally strike a different chord in your
brain.
References:
Sievers, B.,
Polansky, L., Casey, M., & Wheatley, T. (2012). Music and movement share a dynamic
structure that supports universal expressions of emotion. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 110(1), 70–75. doi:10.1073/pnas.1209023110
Here is an explanation of why minor chords sound sad:
ReplyDeletehttps://www.longdom.org/open-access/why-do-minor-chords-sound-sad-the-theory-of-musical-equilibration-andthe-emotions-of-chords-9298.html